Although activating PCx in vivo under our conditions had variable

Although activating PCx in vivo under our conditions had variable effects on spontaneous M/T cell activity, it consistently reduced M/T cell firing

during odor stimulation. The effects of cortical activation on M/T cell responses were also sensitive to odor concentration, consistent with the notion of a synergistic effect between sensory Epigenetic inhibitor input and cortical activity. The increases and decreases in spontaneous activity across different M/T cells suggests that cortically-evoked disynaptic inhibition is sufficient to suppress spontaneous firing in some M/T cells, while others show a net increase in firing presumably due to IPSP-triggered rebound spikes or “disinhibition” mediated by dSACs. The major effect of cortical activation on M/T cell odor responses was a reduction in odor-evoked excitation and an enhancement of odor-evoked inhibition. The augmentation of purely inhibitory responses further implies that cortical activity amplifies lateral

inhibition during sensory processing in the OB. Although cortical fibers target multiple classes of interneurons in the OB, we suspect that cortically-driven GC excitation plays a dominant role during odor processing. In brain slices, tetanic stimulation of the GC layer (Chen et al., 2000; Halabisky and Strowbridge, 2003) or anterior PCx (Balu et al., 2007) has been shown to facilitate mitral cell-evoked recurrent and lateral inhibition. Thus, cortical excitatory input onto GC proximal dendrites could BMS-907351 datasheet contribute to the relief of the Mg2+ block of NMDARs at distal dendrodendritic synapses and boost or “gate” inhibition onto mitral cells (Balu et al., 2007; Halabisky and Strowbridge, 2003; Strowbridge, 2009). Our in vivo findings that cortical input Rutecarpine preferentially drives OB inhibition during sensory processing are in good agreement with this gating model. However, we do not rule out a contribution of glomerular

layer interneurons to the enhancement of odor-evoked inhibition. While GC-mediated inhibition contributes to odor discrimination (Abraham et al., 2010), the role of lateral inhibition in odor coding is controversial. Although it has been proposed to sharpen the odor tuning of M/T cells belonging to individual glomeruli in a center-surround fashion (Yokoi et al., 1995), this requires a chemotopic map such that glomeruli that respond to similar odorant features are spatially clustered. However, studies have highlighted the lack of a fine scale glomerular chemotopic map and found that M/T cells are not preferentially influenced by nearby glomeruli (Fantana et al., 2008; Soucy et al., 2009). Rather than exerting local actions, lateral inhibition could underlie a more uniform reduction in the activity of M/T cells across all glomeruli and act as a gain control mechanism (Soucy et al., 2009).

We found that the DIMD shows a nearly identical activity profile

We found that the DIMD shows a nearly identical activity profile to the DCMD ( Figures 7C and 7D). There was no significant difference in the amplitude of the peak firing rate between the two neurons ( Figure S5A) except at l/|v| = 10 ms. The DCMD peak firing rate, however, occurred slightly earlier than the DIMD for small l/|v| values ( Figure S5B).

The simplest explanation for these results is that the DCMD and the DIMD—given its close resemblance to the DCMD—can interchangeably and equally well mediate jump escape behaviors. According to this hypothesis, because EPSPs elicited in the FETi by these neurons summate, the reduction in jump probability and the increase in variability following nerve cord sectioning would be at least partially click here explained by the absence of one of them, resulting in delayed cocontraction and a smaller number of subsequent extensor spikes. We conclude that the DCMD is not necessary for jump escape behaviors, provided that the

second nerve cord remains intact, check details since the DIMD can presumably take over its role. Next, we selectively ablated the DCMD in one nerve cord by filling it intracellularly with 6-carboxy-fluorescein, a phototoxic dye, and shining laser light onto it (Experimental Procedures). In addition, we sectioned the other nerve cord. This allowed us to determine whether the DCMD is necessary among descending contralateral neurons for the generation of looming-evoked escape behaviors. Since other axons, including the DIMD Ergoloid receiving input from the ipsilateral eye, should remain intact in the spared nerve cord, we used stimulation of the ipsilateral

eye as a control ( Figure 8, inset). We could successfully carry out the ablation procedure in 9 locusts (out of 40 locusts in which the procedure was attempted), as evidenced by the selective disappearance of the DCMD spikes from extracellular recordings in response to looming stimuli (Figures S6A and S6B and Laser Ablation Optical Setup). We could subsequently elicit jumps in four of these locusts. An additional five animals prepared for but did not carry out a jump in response to looming stimuli to either eye. Since these experiments were carried out without a telemetry backpack, we analyzed the jump preparation sequence in these nine locusts based on simultaneously acquired video recordings. The timing of the IJM (see Figure 1 and Figure 3), which is a proxy for the activation onset of flexor motor neurons in intact animals (Fotowat and Gabbiani, 2007), did not differ when stimulating the eye ipsi- or contralateral to the remaining nerve cord. However, it showed higher variability in response to stimulation of the contralateral eye and a lower correlation with l/|v| (Figure 8; ρcontra = 0.48, p = 0.009; ρipsi = 0.69, p < 10−9).

NMDARs have been implicated in numerous cellular processes and ps

NMDARs have been implicated in numerous cellular processes and psychiatric disorders. Studying behavioral effects of NMDAR antagonism in humans has led to the hypothesis that glutamatergic synapse dysfunction may be the root cause of schizophrenia (Belsham, 2001 and Moghaddam, 2003). In addition, suppression of NMDAR function during development recapitulates schizophrenia-like symptoms in adult mice (Stefani and Moghaddam, 2005). Supporting a hypoglutamate hypothesis of schizophrenia, mice with suppressed NMDAR function have been generated as effective animal models of this disorder (Mohn et al., 1999 and Belforte et al., 2010).

One of these, the GluN1 hypomorph mouse, exhibits a phenotype that Dasatinib cell line includes hyperlocomotion and altered sociability. Because of the lethality associated with the GluN2B global knockout mouse, the role of GluN2B in the expression of this behavioral phenotype had not been tested. GluN1 hypomorph animals exhibit a reduction in total NMDAR expression to approximately 5%–10%. In contrast, in 2B→2A homozygous animals, approximately 60% of the integrated cortical NMDAR current is recovered,

but it is now mediated purely by GluN2A-contaning NMDARs. Interestingly, this manipulation recapitulates a behavioral phenotype of hyperlocomotion and altered Ipatasertib research buy sociability. This predicts that aspects of the behavioral phenotype associated with suppressed NMDAR dysfunction in the GluN1 hypomorph may be specifically due to a loss of GluN2B-containing NMDAR signaling during Tryptophan synthase development. We also show that developmental excision of GluN2B in primary cortical and hippocampal neurons (2BΔCtx) recapitulates aspects of the phenotype observed in the 2B→2A animal. It has previously

been shown that suppression of NMDAR function in cortical and hippocampal GABAergic interneurons results in schizophrenia-like symptoms in mice (Belforte et al., 2010). Although future experiments are needed to compare and contrast the phenotypes of these animals, data suggest that changes in corticolimbic excitatory and inhibitory balance may underlie these behavioral alterations. In summary, our experiments show that GluN2B is a critical regulator of homeostatic synaptic plasticity and social behavior. They demonstrate that GluN2A, in spite of being functionally expressed in the absence of GluN2B at cortical synapses, is unable to rescue GluN2B loss of function. We infer that a major function of GluN2B-containing NMDARs is to suppress local protein translation in dendrites through interaction with downstream signaling molecules, including alpha-CaMKII and the mTOR pathway.

, 2009) It is essential to shift focus from etiology to the reac

, 2009). It is essential to shift focus from etiology to the reaction of the nervous system to the etiological pathology—to viewing neuropathic pain as a manifestation of pathological neural plasticity. The advantage of this approach is that it will lead to an explicit dual therapeutic focus aimed both at etiological factors and the forms of maladaptive plasticity they initiate. By definition, neuropathic pain involves damage to the nervous system (Jensen et al., 2011). Often, negative symptoms are the first indication of damage to the somatosensory system and can be detected by quantitative sensory testing as well as clinical examination and to a more limited extent, history/questionnaire.

The cause of negative symptoms in peripheral neuropathies is direct insult to primary sensory neurons. This may produce cell death or compromise transduction (due to terminal atrophy) or conduction (due to loss selleck products of peripheral axons) or transmission (due to loss of central terminals) of sensory information. Loss of function can manifest across the whole sensory spectrum (e.g., global numbness after a traumatic nerve injury) or it can affect specific modalities (Freeman, 2009). For example, an elevated heat threshold due to degeneration of intraepithelial C-fibers is a common early manifestation of peripheral diabetic DNA Damage inhibitor neuropathy

(Said, 2007) and in chemotherapy-induced neuropathies, where sensory but not motor axons show mitochondrial damage leading to hypoesthesia (Xiao et al., 2011). Many patients with neural damage only have negative symptoms, Phosphatidylinositol diacylglycerol-lyase some though, also have positive symptoms because particular pathological processes are engaged that increase pain sensitivity or drive spontaneous activation of the nociceptive pathway. Peripheral sensitization most characteristically occurs after peripheral inflammation and comprises a reduction in threshold and an increase in the excitability of the peripheral terminals of nociceptors in response to sensitizing inflammatory mediators. This results in innocuous stimuli at the

site of inflammation, such as light touch, warm or cool temperatures, being perceived as painful (allodynia), and stimuli that usually are felt as uncomfortable or slightly painful, such as a pinprick, becoming extremely painful (hyperalgesia) in the primary area of inflammation. However, peripheral sensitization can also occur after nerve lesions in the presence (peripheral neuritis) and absence of tissue inflammation, and thereby can contribute to pain hypersensitivity within the innervation zone of an affected nerve (Figure 3). External mechanical, thermal, and chemical stimuli are converted into voltage changes in sensory neurons by ion channels that respond to specific environmental stimuli. After nerve injury, peripheral sensitization results from reduced thresholds for activation these transducer channels together with nerve injury induced changes in sodium and potassium channels.

In the forced swim test, mutant animals during the acute phase sp

In the forced swim test, mutant animals during the acute phase spend less time immobile on day 2 (Figure 8B), suggesting that they

are hyper-reactive and more anxious about the water-swim stress. These results suggest that shortly after cell ablation causes mossy cell degeneration, granule-cell excitability increases, eliciting anxiety-like behaviors. To MS-275 order address whether mossy cell degeneration leads to impaired hippocampus-dependent learning, we subjected DT-treated mutants and control littermates to two contextual discrimination paradigms. Mice in acute and chronic DT treatment phases were subjected to a one-trial contextual fear-conditioning test to assess whether, 3 hr and 24 hr after conditioning, mutants can discriminate shocking context A from a modality-different context B (Figure S5D). Whether in acute (Figure 8C) or chronic (Figure 8D) phases of DT-treatment, mutants and controls show similar freezing levels before and immediately after shock during conditioning, indicating mossy cell ablation has no impact on contextual fear learning. On the recall test, however, mutant animals in the acute phase (but not chronic-phase mutants or controls) are unable to distinguish context A from context

B 3 hr (genotype-context interaction F(1,38) = 3.1, p < 0.05; Newman-Keuls post hoc test, p < 0.05 for control, p = 0.42 for mutant) and 24 hr (Figure 8C) after conditioning. Notably, when mice are tested in the chronic phase of DT exposure, this impairment disappears both at 3 hr (context effect, F(1,30) = 24.1, p < 0.01; Newman-Keuls post hoc test, Selleck FG-4592 p < 0.005 for control, p < 0.01 for mutant) and 24 hr (Figure 8D) after conditioning. Contextual discrimination impairment in mutants therefore appears to occur only in the acute phase of DT exposure, when granule cell excitability is highest. To investigate whether mutants’ inability to discriminate contexts is consistent across tasks, we subjected naive animals with found acute DT exposure to a contextual step-through

active avoidance task. In the initial latency test crossing from light to dark compartments, no difference was detected among genotypes before conditioning (42.4 ± 11.3 s for control, 58.3 ± 24.0 s for mutant, t test, p = 0.54). In context X, mice entering the dark compartment received a single foot shock (0.12 mA, 2 s). Twenty-four hours after conditioning, the mice were placed back in the dark compartment, either in the non-shock context Y or in the US-associated context X (Figure S5E). Reverse latency to escape from the dark compartment (X or Y) was measured for each context. Control mice had longer escape latency from safe context Y, while this was not seen in the mutants (Figure 8E). These results confirm that in the acute phase of mossy cell degeneration, mutants’ recall for a fear memory in a specific context is impaired.

, 2010) hpo-30 mutants display a striking asymmetric defect in w

, 2010). hpo-30 mutants display a striking asymmetric defect in which the majority of PVD lateral branches are restricted to the right side ( Figure 7H), and most of these fasciculate with motor neuron commissures ( Figure 7I). Thus, hpo-30 appears to function largely in commissure-independent stabilization of lateral branches. This analysis defines two mechanisms of dendrite stabilization, one that requires HPO-30 and is not associated with the commissures and a separate pathway that utilizes a different protein for fasciculation

with motor neuron commissures ( Smith et al., 2010). HPO-30 is also likely to support higher order PVD branching since the residual 2° branches in hpo-30 mutants do not result in recognizable menorahs with a full complement of 3° and 4° dendrites ( Figures 7 and 8). The frequent occurrence of overlapping PVD dendrites selleck compound in the hpo-30 mutant ( Figures 7A and 7H) is suggestive of an additional role in dendrite self-avoidance. Because HPO-30 is required

for PVD dendritic branching, we hypothesized that HPO-30 is also necessary for branching of the extra PVD-like cell, cAVM, in ahr-1 mutants. This idea was substantiated by the finding that cAVM lateral branches were largely eliminated in ahr-1;hpo-30 double mutants ( Figures 8C and 8D). To ask if AHR-1 regulates HPO-30, we visualized hpo-30::GFP in an ahr-1 mutant background and confirmed that hpo-30::GFP is ectopically expressed in cAVM ( Figure S7). These results indicate that AHR-1 blocks expression of HPO-30 to prevent touch neurons from adopting the lateral branching architecture of the PVD neuron ( Figure 6K). Reduced hpo-30::GFP expression in Galunisertib cAVM in an ahr-1;mec-3 double mutant confirmed that mec-3 function is necessary for ectopic hpo-30::GFP expression in cAVM in an ahr-1 mutant background Carnitine dehydrogenase (data not shown). This effect is also consistent with our finding that mec-3

promotes hpo-30::GFP expression in PVD ( Figure 7D). Thus, our results are indicative of a transcriptional mechanism in touch neurons ( Figure 6K) in which ahr-1 activates mec-3 while simultaneously blocking expression of hpo-30, a mec-3 target gene that promotes lateral branching. Having shown that hpo-30 function is required for the PVD-like dendritic morphology of cAVM ( Figure 8D), we next asked if hpo-30 expression was sufficient to induce lateral branching in wild-type light touch neurons. Normally, touch neurons adopt a simple, unbranched morphology ( Figures 1 and 8). Ectopic expression of HPO-30 in PLM with the mec-4 promoter, however, resulted in the appearance of aberrant lateral branches that are not observed in the wild-type ( Figure 8F). AVM and PVM did not show ectopic branches in this experiment, but their longitudinal processes are located in the ventral nerve cord ( Figure 1) and thus are not in contact with the epidermal region in which HPO-30 normally promotes PVD branching.

Though cochlear implantation has profoundly influenced our treatm

Though cochlear implantation has profoundly influenced our treatment of children with congenital deafness, there are still significant limitations in function with an implant, and these results cannot compare to native hearing (Kral and O’Donoghue, 2010). Thus, there remains intense interest in restoring normal organ of Corti function through techniques

such as hair cell regeneration and gene therapy (Di Domenico et al., 2011). To date, a majority of the research in this arena has focused on cochlear hair cell regeneration, applicable to the most common forms of hearing loss including presbycusis, noise damage, infection, and ototoxicity. Several studies have now demonstrated regeneration of hair cells in injured mice cochlea and improvement of both hearing and balance with virally mediated delivery of Math1 ( Baker et al., Rapamycin supplier 2009, Husseman and Raphael, 2009, Izumikawa et al., 2008, Kawamoto et al., 2003, Praetorius et al., 2010 and Staecker et al., 2007). While these efforts in wild-type animals are quite important, they still do not address the problem of an underlying causative genetic mutation. In such a scenario, even successfully regenerated hair cells will still ZD1839 mouse be subject to the innate genetic mutation that led to

hair cell loss in the first place. To date, efforts to restore hearing in this type of hearing loss with gene therapy have been met with limited success ( Maeda et al., 2009), and no study has reported the reversal of deafness in an animal model of genetic deafness. Previous reports have described a mouse model of hereditary deafness, which occurs as a result of a null mutation in the gene coding for the vesicular glutamate transporter-3 (VGLUT3) (Obholzer et al., 2008, Ruel et al., 2008 and Seal et al., 2008). Synaptic transmission mediated by glutamate requires transport of the excitatory amino acid into secretory vesicles by a family of three vesicular glutamate transporters (Fremeau

old et al., 2004 and Takamori et al., 2002). We previously demonstrated that inner hair cells of the cochlea express VGLUT3 and that mice lacking this transporter are congenitally deaf (Seal et al., 2008). Hearing loss in these mice is due to the elimination of glutamate release by inner hair cells and hence to the loss of synaptic transmission at the IHC-afferent nerve synapse. Subsequent studies have shown that a missense mutation in the human gene SLC17A8, which encodes VGLUT3, might underlie the progressive high-frequency hearing loss seen in autosomal dominant DFNA25 (Ruel et al., 2008). Here we report the successful restoration of hearing in the VGLUT3 knockout (KO) mouse using virally mediated gene delivery.

8A) No such increase was observed in the pCIneo group This incr

8A). No such increase was observed in the pCIneo group. This increase in the %Tg preceded cell division as no CFSE dye dilution was observed by d3 (data not shown). We speculate that this is indicative of retention of Eα-specific T cells or inhibition of T cell egress from the lymphoid tissues, due to stable APC-T cell interactions as we [22], and others [23] have noted in other T cell priming regimes. There was no corresponding increase in the percentage of non-Tg CD4+ T cells in draining LNs (Fig. 8A), distal peripheral LNs or spleen (data not shown), suggesting that the TEa EPZ-6438 cost accumulation we

observed was Ag-driven. Concomitantly, we observed significant blastogenesis of Eα-specific T cells, in all tissues of pCI-EαRFP and pCI-EαGFP-immunised mice (Fig. 8A). No TEa blasts check details were found in pCIneo-immunised groups. These results are strongly suggestive of presentation Eα peptide to Eα-specific CD4+ T cells at d3 following plasmid vaccination and that T cells in the draining, and distal LNs and spleen have seen Ag by this time. In order to determine if there were any differences in the kinetics of T cell activation in these anatomically distinct lymphoid tissues, we analysed cell

division history using adoptive transfer of CFSE-labelled TEa T cells. By d5 we observed Eα-specific T cell division in draining lymph nodes, but little division in more distal peripheral LNs and the spleen (Fig. 8B and C). However by d10 we found TEa division in all lymphoid tissues examined, with the highest proportion of divided cells being found in the spleen. Thus although the T cell response to pDNA-encoded Ag appears to commence in the local draining lymph nodes, this is superceded by responses in the spleen. We also examined intermediate timepoints, and have never observed

the multiple division peaks, typically found when using CFSE for T cell proliferation, suggesting that the Eα-specific T cells had divided in a different location and Tryptophan synthase once divided had migrated to the tissues examined, or that very few naïve re-circulating T cells synchronously enter cell division, presumably due to limiting amounts of Ag. Only when they have divided more than 6 times have they accumulated sufficiently for us to detect cell division. We were unable to find evidence for Ag presentation at timepoints other than d3. These results correlate with the appearance of pMHC complexes in draining lymph nodes, hence from our data it appears that Ag presentation peaks 3 days after DNA immunisation.

, 2012), leaving uncertainty regarding

the respective con

, 2012), leaving uncertainty regarding

the respective contributions find more of these factors to the development of hypertension. Asians, a racial/ethnic group with a high prevalence of hypertension (Kearney et al., 2005 and Kubo et al., 2008), are particularly understudied regarding this issue. Therefore, the purpose of the present study was to investigate the independent association of the presence of proteinuria and a reduced eGFR with incident hypertension in a prospective cohort study of young to middle-aged Japanese males with annual BP evaluation. The study subjects included Japanese males who underwent annual medical checkups at their workplaces, all of which were blue-chip companies in Japan (Kondo et al., 2013 and Yamashita et al., 2012). Japanese males 16–59 years of

age (n = 33,914) were recruited in 2000. We excluded participants with preexisting hypertension (systolic BP ≥ 140 mm Hg, diastolic BP ≥ 90 mm Hg or the use of antihypertensive drugs; n = 4688 at baseline examination) and excluded participants aged < 18 years old (n = 45), with a final sample of 29,181 participants. Annual medical checkups including blood test and dipstick urine test were conducted through 2010 or until retirement at around 60 years of age. All participants were individually interviewed using a structured questionnaire in the baseline and annual follow-up surveys. The following information was recorded by trained observers: smoking status, alcohol intake, medical first history and medications. The smoking status and alcohol intake were classified as current vs. former/never. Weight and height were measured while the subject was wearing light Quisinostat cell line clothing without shoes. The body mass index (BMI) was computed as the weight in kilograms divided by the square of the height in meters. Urine and blood samples were obtained in the morning with overnight

fasting. A urinalysis for proteinuria was conducted with Uropaper III (Eiken Chemical Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan), and the results were measured using a US-2100 Automated Urine Analyzer (trace (±) corresponds to proteinuria ≥ 15 mg/dl, 1 + to ≥ 30 mg/dl, 2 + to ≥ 100 mg/dl, 3 + to ≥ 300 mg/dl and 4 + to ≥ 1000 mg/dl). The blood analyses were conducted at a single laboratory. The GFR was estimated using the three-variable equation proposed by the Japanese Society of Nephrology (eGFR [ml/min/1.73 m2] = 194 × serum creatinine− 1.094 × age− 0.287 × 0.739 [if female]) (Matsuo et al., 2009). In this study, the proteinuria using a dipstick and eGFR were measured at baseline (2000). Diabetes mellitus was defined as a concentration of serum fasting glucose of ≥ 126 mg/dl or the use of glucose-lowering medications. BP was measured annually with the participant in the sitting position after 5 min of rest using an automated sphygmomanometer (BP-203IIIB; Colin Corporation, Tokyo, Japan). The BP was measured two times at intervals of 1 min on the right arm, and the average value was calculated as the baseline BP.

Behaviors associated with food seeking, recognition, and ingestio

Behaviors associated with food seeking, recognition, and ingestion can be categorized as appetitive versus consummatory, according to traditional analyses of animal behavior (Kupfermann, Carfilzomib 1974a; Lorenz, 1950). The first category corresponds to exploratory behavior that is subject to environmental influence and that may displays variability. The consummatory category

refers to the release and execution of innate behavioral sequences and its display is more or less invariant. Single neuropeptides (like neuropeptide Y [NPY]) contribute to both appetitive and consummatory feeding behaviors in mammals (Dailey and Bartness, 2009), and their roles in the fundamental neuronal circuits underlying feeding behaviors have been intensively studied. For example, detailed models are now emerging the explain how peptidergic neurons in the arcuate nucleus that secrete Agouti-related peptide (AgRP), GABA, and NPY promote feeding by inhibiting other neurons of the parabrachial, arcuate, and paraventricular nuclei of the hypothalamus (Aponte et al., 2011; Atasoy et al., 2012; Wu et al., 2009). That action depends on GABA and NPY

more than AgRP, and is age-dependent and subject to hormonal modulation (Yang et al., 2011) (Luquet et al., 2005). To complement such advancing mammalian studies, invertebrate model systems offer sophisticated PD0325901 concentration genetic manipulation and/or favorable cellular resolution: these features can help address the basis for the profound effects peptide modulators have on feeding behavior. In this section, we overview several different invertebrate studies (in insects, in Caenorhabditis elegans, in Aplysia) to illustrate potential cellular mechanisms of how peptide modulation may contribute to shape both appetitive and consummatory feeding behaviors. The motivational state profoundly influences the specific responses animals display in response to identical, food-associated stimuli (Kupfermann, 1974b). In Drosophila, neuropeptides implicated

in regulating feeding-associated behaviors include the NPY homolog, NPF ( Krashes et al., 2009; Wu et al., 2005a, 2005b), which, like NPY, appears to be specifically dedicated to modulation of circuits involved only in metabolism, stress, and energy homeostasis ( Nässel and Wegener, 2011). Other feeding-associated peptides include hugin ( Melcher and Pankratz, 2005), leukokinin ( Al-Anzi et al., 2010), and allatostatin A ( Hergarden et al., 2012). For example, starvation increases food-searching behavior by the fly and increases physiological responsiveness in an identified olfactory glomerulus called DM1. DM1 normally responds to cider vinegar and its state-dependent responsiveness is increased due to the actions of a neuropeptide called small NPF (sNPF). sNPF is genetically distinct from NPF, is found at all levels of the neuraxis, and is probably involved in many diverse modulatory functions ( Nässel and Wegener, 2011).