, 1968 and Schneider and Sherman, 1968) In 2000, Nader and colle

, 1968 and Schneider and Sherman, 1968). In 2000, Nader and colleagues raised this challenge again in experiments that targeted the known role of the amygdala in synaptic consolidation of the Pavlovian association of a tone (CS) with a shock (US; Falls et al., 1992 and Duvarci et al., 2006), showing that a CS alone reminder presented long after consolidation HIF-1 activation was complete re-engaged the temporary susceptibility of the memory. These findings were interpreted as evidence that the reminder reactivated the original

memory trace, making it necessary to “reconsolidate” the memory, or else suffer erasure of the memory (Sara, 2000 and Nader et al., 2000). Over the last decade, many experiments have supported the observation of memory

susceptibility following reminders and these findings have been reviewed extensively in recent papers (Nader and Hardt, 2009, Dudai and Eisenberg, 2004, Lee, 2010, Alberini, 2011 and Sara, 2010). Results supporting the existence of reconsolidation have been reported in several species across a broad range of learning tests, and using a variety of manipulations to block memory (e.g., Rose and Rankin, 2006, Pedreira et al., 2002, Eisenberg et al., 2003, Frankland et al., 2006, Lee et al., 2005, Hupbach et al., 2007, Monfils et al., 2009 and Schiller Bioactive Compound Library order et al., 2010). Despite broad support for the generality of reconsolidation (Nader and Hardt, 2009), several studies have failed to find Megestrol Acetate that amnesic agents block memory in the reconsolidation paradigm (Biedenkapp and Rudy, 2004) or have observed that

the memory deficits are temporary (Lattal and Abel, 2004 and Power et al., 2006), leading to the idea that the reconsolidation phenomenon has “boundary conditions” (Eisenberg et al., 2003, Milekic and Alberini, 2002 and Morris et al., 2006). Several experimental parameters have been shown to be important in determining whether reconsolidation occurs, including how memories are reactivated (Debiec et al., 2006 and Tronel et al., 2005), whether novelty is introduced during memory reactivation (Pedreira et al., 2004), and the age and strength of a memory (Eisenberg et al., 2003 and Milekic and Alberini, 2002). We consider two main categories of boundary conditions: which memory is active at the time of amnesic treatment and whether the reminder generates new learning. Early in the recent series of studies on reconsolidation there were conflicting reports on whether reminders reinstated lability of memories for classical aversive conditioning. Several studies (Berman et al., 2003, Vianna et al.

Accordingly, it is important that computational models themselves

Accordingly, it is important that computational models themselves be HDAC inhibitor grounded in sound neural engineering principles that impact how the models are framed, implemented, and

interpreted ( Eliasmith and Anderson, 2004 and Eliasmith, 2013). I gratefully acknowledge contributions of the many people in my lab who helped develop and apply the brain mapping tools we have generated over the past two decades. Also, the contributions of colleagues in the Human Connectome Project have been hugely important and are greatly appreciated. I thank Matt Glasser and Sandra Curtiss for comments on the manuscript and Susan Danker for assistance in manuscript preparation. This work is supported by NIH grant MH60974 and by 1U54MH091657, funded by the 16 NIH Institutes and Centers that support the NIH Blueprint for Neuroscience Research. “
“The study of decision making occurs within psychology, statistics, economics, finance, engineering (e.g., quality control), political science, philosophy, medicine, ethics, and jurisprudence. The neuroscience behind decision making touches on only a fraction

of these areas, although it is a frequent source of delight when a connection emerges between neural mechanisms and each of these areas. While decision making, per se, fascinates, what makes the neuroscience of decision making special is the Protease Inhibitor Library research buy insight it promises on a deeper topic. For the neurobiology of decision making is really the neurobiology of cognition—or at the very least a large component of cognition that is tractable to experimental neuroscience. It exposes principles of neural processing that underlie a variety of mental functions. Moreover,

we believe these same principles, enumerated below, will furnish critical insight into the pathophysiology of diseases that compromise cognitive function, and ultimately they will supply the key to ameliorating cognitive dysfunction. For this special issue of Neuron’s 25th anniversary, we focus on a line no of research that began almost exactly 25 years ago, in the laboratory of Bill Newsome. It is an honor to share our perspective on the field: its roots, an overview of the progress we have made, and some ideas about some of the directions we might pursue in the next 25 years. Approximately 25 years ago, Bill Newsome, Ken Britten, and Tony Movshon recorded from neurons in extrastriate area MT/V5 of rhesus monkeys while those monkeys performed a demanding direction discrimination task. They made two important discoveries. First, the fidelity of the single-neuron response to motion rivaled the fidelity of the monkey’s behavioral reports, in other words, choice accuracy. The fidelity of a neural response is a characterization of the relationship between its signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) and the stimulus difficulty level.

However, the area of pERK positive fibers per blood vessel was co

However, the area of pERK positive fibers per blood vessel was comparable between Trpv4−/− mice and controls (compare Figure 7B with Figure 7C). It may be that basal activation of pERK in sensory fibers is already altered in the absence of TRPV4 channels. Together these results indicate that the presence of TRPV4 is necessary for hypo-osmotic stimuli to activate hepatic osmoreceptors. To gauge the potential influence of Compound Library hepatic afferents in regulating blood osmolality in humans, we conducted an investigation of a large cohort of liver transplant recipients. We found that the blood osmolality of the liver transplant

group (n = 40, age range 21–74 years) was slightly (∼2 mOsm), but significantly (p < 0.05 Student`s t test) elevated compared to a control age Adriamycin manufacturer matched cohort (n = 57, age range 23–61 years) (Figure 7E). The levels of the C-terminal pro-arginine-vasopressin (copeptin) were also significantly elevated in samples from liver transplantees (15 ± 4.8 pmol/l) compared to healthy controls (4.16 ± 0.4 pmol/l; p < 0.01, Student's t test). Copeptin is a stable marker of vasopressin levels, which is upregulated in patients with experimentally induced

increases in blood osmolality (Szinnai et al., 2007), the upregulation seen here suggests normal central osmoregulation in liver transplantees. These findings, which will be followed up in a more detailed longitudinal clinical study, suggest that hepatic afferents may also contribute to human osmoregulation. Here, we have identified a specific population of TRPV4-positive hepatic sensory afferents that detect physiological changes in blood osmolality. We show that the in vivo activation of hepatic afferents by physiological changes in hepatic portal vein osmolality is absent Thymidine kinase in Trpv4−/− mutant mice. Strikingly, hepatic thoracic sensory neurons possess a fast and sensitive osmosensitive current that can dynamically signal

physiologically relevant hypo-osmotic shifts in blood osmolality. We have also shown that identified small hepatic sensory neurons completely lack the osmosensitive current in Trpv4−/− mice. The pharmacological and biophysical characteristics of the osmosensitive current in hepatic afferents together with the genetic evidence, suggests that this current is, at least in part, mediated by TRPV4 channels ( Figure 2 and Figure 6). In summary, we have characterized, in detail, the cellular and molecular properties of a novel population of hepatic afferents that in humans probably forms the afferent arm of an important regulatory reflex involved in the regulation of metabolism, blood pressure, and osmolality homeostasis. Peripheral sensory neurons that function as osmosensors have received little attention, although there is literature indicating their existence in animals (Adachi, 1984, Adachi et al.

To identify

To identify see more our stimulus-specific ROIs, we utilized the localizer data. To create object-sensitive ROIs, we used the object > scene localizer contrast. Three scene localizer ROIs, created from the scene > object localizer contrast, were also created but only the outcome of analyses utilizing the left PPA seed (center at −24, −43, −2) are reported. The right PPA (center at 21,

−34, −5) and right retrosplenial cortex seeds (center at −21, −52, 19) demonstrated qualitatively similar results to those of the left PPA and are available from the authors by request. Given the extensive activation of the medial temporal lobe in the localizer contrasts (>500 voxels), both peaks and subpeaks of activity within our regions of interest were utilized as central points in the generation of ROI spheres (each with

a 5 mm radius; see Supplemental Information for the peak and subpeak coordinates of effects identified in these contrasts). The seeds were generated such that only active voxels within each sphere for a given contrast were included in the ROI. In the event that any voxels were shared between two generated PD0325901 solubility dmso ROIs, shared voxels were removed from each of the relevant seeds. From the object localizer contrast, we report the outcome of analyses involving two seeds, one in left perirhinal cortex (center at −33, −4, −32) and the other in right perirhinal cortex (center at 33, −7, −29). An additional seed was created around a left hippocampal peak (center at −33, −19, −14) from the object > scene contrast, but given that this seed overlapped with Mephenoxalone the stimulus-general seed, and results arising from its use were roughly identical to those when the stimulus-general seed was employed, we do not report the outcome of general analyses utilizing this seed. See Figure 3A for a depiction of the reported ROIs on the mean anatomical image. Importantly, it should be noted that all ROIs were created from independent data from the conditions that were utilized in the main beta series correlation analyses (see Kriegeskorte et al., 2009). Additionally, given that the ROIs were not created with respect to behavioral performance in the conditions for which the analyses

were conducted (LD object, SD object, LD scene, and SD scene trials), the correlational analysis with behavioral performance reported in the Results section is also not subject to a nonindependence issue. This work was funded by NIMH RO1–MH074692 and Dart Neuroscience to L.D. “
“Wouldn’t it be nice to know what would have happened if you had chosen differently? Imagine driving on a highway toward a traffic jam faced with two choices: bypass the highway or wait in the hold up. Neither of the cases provides information about which decision really yields the better result. On the other hand, when choosing between two lanes in a traffic jam, you will always notice the progress you are making in your lane and the progress you could have been making in the other lane. Both humans (Burke et al.

, 2007, Kutner et al , 2004 and Law et al , 2005) We analyzed th

, 2007, Kutner et al., 2004 and Law et al., 2005). We analyzed the monkey LFP data using the same multiple linear regression β weight analysis used on the human BOLD fMRI signals, examining nonoverlapping frequency bandwidths in the gamma (30–100 Hz) and beta (10–25 Hz) ranges derived from spectral analyses (Figures S1A and S1B available online). In some selleck chemicals cases where there was not enough data available to carry out a multiple regression analysis, we used standard parametric statistics to analyze the LFP data. The results of each analysis were compared across species to identify similarities as well as differences

in the neurophysiological responses. A common finding from the monkey entorhinal cortex has been strong responses to relatively novel stimuli (stimuli seen for the first time in the current session) compared to highly familiar stimuli (significant exposure over multiple days to months) (Brown et al., 1987, Suzuki et al., 1997 and Xiang and Brown, 1998). Few if any such signals have been reported in the hippocampus (Brown et al., 1987 and Xiang and Brown, 1998). We first asked whether

differences in responses to new versus highly familiar stimuli could be found in the monkey LFP signals. LFP sweeps were converted to frequency spectra and the mean log power from both the beta and selleck products gamma bandwidths of a 1,100 ms epoch spanning the scene and delay periods

were derived. The spectral power values from the selected bandwidths were then analyzed with multiple regressions for each session to generate β values for both the new and the highly familiar reference stimuli. These β values were then compared across sessions using parametric tests. For the monkey entorhinal cortex, significant differences between new and reference β values were found for the beta bandwidth (t(52) = 5.69; p < 0.0005), but not the gamma bandwidth (t(52) = 0.323; p = ns; Figure 2A). The direction of the effect in the beta bandwidth favored reference Metalloexopeptidase over new trial spectra. During separate recording sessions in the monkey hippocampus, significant differences in β values were found for both the beta (t(39) = 3.15; p < 0.003) and the gamma (t(39) = 2.35; p < 0.024) bandwidths. Additional analyses done to examine the detailed structure of signal showed that the differential signals we observed arose from a transient decrease during the scene/delay period relative to the fixation period that was larger (more negative) for the new conditions than for the reference conditions (Figures S1C–S1F). In humans, we applied a multiple regression analysis of the fMRI data calculating coefficients for the new and reference trial responses for each subject. The β values reflected the difference in activity between mnemonic and non-mnemonic tasks for each voxel.

BRP forms macromolecular assemblies that are involved in shaping

BRP forms macromolecular assemblies that are involved in shaping the T bar (Fouquet et al., 2009). Several “BRP strands” join at their N-terminal

ends and contact Cacophony calcium channels near the presynaptic membrane Raf inhibitor (Kittel et al., 2006), while BRP C-terminal ends extend into the cytoplasm (Figure 3H). BRPNC82 antibodies label the BRP C-terminal portion, while anti-BRPN antibodies label the N-terminal end of the protein (Fouquet et al., 2009). To further quantify the defect in BRPNC82 labeling, we measured dot number in controls and elp3 mutants. As shown in Figure 3J, we do not observe an increase in the number of BRPNC82 dots per boutonic area in elp3 mutants, and similarly, we also do not find a difference in the densities of dots per bouton of other active zone markers including LIP and CAC in elp3 mutants and controls ( Figures 3B, 3C, 3L, and 3M), indicating that elp3 mutations do not affect the number of active zones per synaptic area. Furthermore, compared to controls, we also do not observe altered calcium influx measured using GCaMP3 ( Tian et al., 2009)

in elp3 mutant boutons ( Figures S2E–S2G), in line with normal calcium channel clustering and function in the mutants. Next, we quantified BRPNC82 dot size (maximum diameter) in controls and elp3 null mutants and found an overall increase in the size of individual BRPNC82 dots ( Figures 3D and 3N), suggesting increased immunoreactivity of this antigen at individual active zones. To scrutinize the BRP defect in elp3 mutants in more detail, MLN2238 in vivo we also quantified features of BRPN labeling in

controls and elp3 mutants ( Figures 3F, 3H, 3K, and 3O). First, we quantified the number of BRPN dots per bouton area but did not find a difference, again indicating that ELP3 does not affect the number of active zones per bouton area ( Figure 3K). Next, we also quantified BRPN dot size, but in contrast to BRPNC82 labeling, BRP dot size revealed by BRPN is very similar at elp3 mutant boutons and controls ( Figure 3O), suggesting that T bar assembly per se (the number of BRP molecules) is not whatever affected in elp3 mutants. We further assessed if in elp3 mutants supernumerous “BRP strands” join ( Figure 3H), by also performing western blots of elp3 mutant and control brains probed with different BRP antibodies but found very similar BRP levels ( Figure 3I; data not shown). Thus, the data indicate normal assembly of BRP strands at active zones and are consistent with morphological alterations at the C-terminal of BRP resulting in a more accessible BRPNC82 epitope in elp3 mutants. To directly assess active zone morphology in elp3 mutant and controls, we performed transmission electron microscopy (TEM).

While our model predicts temporarily precise, strong, and brief M

While our model predicts temporarily precise, strong, and brief MC response to odors, more complex Selleckchem Trichostatin A temporal responses observed in Shusterman et al., 2011 and Cury and Uchida, 2010 are beyond accurate description by our model, mainly due to oversimplified temporal profile of the stimulus. In a more realistic situation, the sniff dynamics controls the raise of the odorant concentration at the epithelium, and different receptors are activated at different phases of the sniffing cycle. The mechanism suggested here could be implemented in other parts of the nervous system and in other species. Thus, the inhibitory interneurons of the insect antennal lobe

(Assisi et al., 2011) could form representations of odorant-dependent inputs of the projection neurons

JQ1 mw in a similar way. Our mechanisms could also be implemented on the basis of axon-dendrite synapses if the symmetry in the synaptic strength between excitatory and inhibitory neurons (Equation 1) is approximately satisfied. This model could therefore be implemented by cortical networks. We theoretically address the experimental evidence of spatially sparse odor codes carried by MCs in awake rodents. We propose a novel role for the GCs of the olfactory bulb in which they collectively build an incomplete representation of the odorants in the inhibitory currents returned to the MCs. These inhibitory currents lead to the balance between excitation and inhibition and suppression of responses however for most of the MCs. Because the representation formed by GCs is incomplete, a small number of MCs can carry information to the cortex, leading to the sparse olfactory codes. Our model predicts sharp transient responses in a large population of MCs. This function is facilitated by the network architecture that includes bidirectional dendrodendritic MC-to-GC synapses. Our model is based on the following equations describing the responses of MCs and GCs, rm and ai: equation(Equation 5) rm=xm−∑i=1NWmiai, equation(Equation 6) u˙i=−ui+∑m=1MW˜imrm,and equation(Equation 7) ai=g(ui),ai=g(ui),where

ui   is the membrane voltage of the GC and rm=ym−y¯m. Here, ym   and y¯m are instantaneous, and average activity of the MC number m  , u˙=du/dt. We assume therefore that the dynamics of MC responses is fast enough to reflect the values of inputs instantly. The description in terms of differential (Equation 5), (Equation 6) and (Equation 7) is equivalent to the minimization of the Lyapunov function: equation(Equation 8) L(a→)=12ε∑m=1M(xm−∑iWmiai)2+∑i=1NC(ai),where C(a) the cost function is defined as equation(Equation 9) C(a)=∫0ag−1(a′)da′. The temporal behavior of the system can be viewed as a form of gradient descent; i.e., u˙i=−∂L/∂ai. The time derivative of the Lyapunov function LL is always negative: equation(Equation 10) L˙=∑i∂L∂aia˙i=∑i(−u˙i)a˙i=−∑i[g−1(ai)]′[a˙i]2≤0.

Model simulations also revealed that the

cbDDM provided a

Model simulations also revealed that the

cbDDM provided a significantly better fit than a stochastic model with collapsing Wnt tumor bounds, when tested against our data from 11 subjects (p = 0.0044, paired t test). With data across two experiments suggesting that humans integrate perceptual evidence over time, we next sought to characterize where this integration occurs in the brain. Although information might be expected to accumulate linearly over time, when the cbDDM is used to simulate the mean accumulated signal for trials of different lengths, it is evident that the time course of integration is nonlinear, increasing more rapidly closer to the time of decision (Figure 5A). Therefore, the behaviorally derived parameters from the cbDDM (including drift rate, diffusion coefficient, and collapse rate) were used, on a subject-by-subject basis, to model the expected temporal profile of information integration. These in turn were used to generate subject-specific fMRI regressors

of interest in an event-related finite-impulse-response ABT-888 chemical structure (FIR) model, enabling us to characterize within-trial temporal changes in the fMRI time series. Note that the absolute value of the integration profile was used to represent evidence toward either decision bound, and only trials of three, four, and five sniffs were included to ensure that sufficient numbers of trials across subjects were available for estimating the imaging data. This approach revealed significant bilateral activity in centromedial OFC (p < 0.05 small-volume corrected), near the anterior-medial portion of area 13l, (following the nomenclature of Ongür et al., Oxygenase 2003), and situated within the putative human olfactory OFC (Gottfried and Zald, 2005) (Figure 5B). To characterize the temporal profile of these activations as a function of trial length, deconvolution techniques (Glover, 1999; Zelano et al., 2009) were used to remove the low-pass effect of the fMRI hemodynamic response function on the mean time series in OFC. These plots show that activity increased at

slower rates for longer trials, peaked at the time of decision, and had lower peaks for longer trials, suggestive of collapsing bounds (Figures 5C and 5D). Statistical analyses demonstrated a main effect of time (sniff number) in OFC (right mOFC, p = 0.007; left mOFC, p = 0.021; repeated-measures ANOVA) and a significant interaction between condition and time in right mOFC (p = 0.032) and at trend level in left mOFC (p = 0.081), demonstrating faster rates of increase for shorter trials. Additionally, a leave-one-subject-out cross-validation technique (Kriegeskorte et al., 2009) was used to obtain unbiased estimates of peak voxel activity in left and right OFC, and resulted in similar time series responses (Figure S2; Supplemental Experimental Procedures). These patterns conform closely to the temporal profiles predicted from the cbDDM model (cf. Figure 5A) and are consistent with olfactory information accumulation in human OFC.

Thus, TRPV1 antagonists might enhance tonic activity in icilin/co

Thus, TRPV1 antagonists might enhance tonic activity in icilin/cold-responsive spinal neurons and enhance behavioral sensitivity to cold (although enhanced behavioral sensitivity to cold may require inactivating not just TRPV1+ neurons but also CGRPα+/TRPV1− neurons). Furthermore, if TRPV1 antagonists enhance activity in cold-responsive spinal circuits in humans, this could simultaneously trigger shivering, the percept of “feeling cold,” and homeostatic mechanisms that warm the body, ultimately producing hyperthermia. While PF-06463922 manufacturer TRPV1 antagonists cause hyperthermia in rodents, CGRPα DRG neuron-ablated mice showed neither hyperthermia nor hypothermia at baseline (Figure 6C). This difference could be due to the fact that it takes several

days for phenotypes to develop after the first DTX injection (for example, see Figure 4). In contrast, TRPV1 antagonists have a rapid onset. Moreover, ablation caused the permanent loss of neurons, which could produce phenotypes that are more typical of sustained TRPV1 antagonism. For example, the hyperthermic response to TRPV1 antagonists eventually dissipates when these antagonists are administered over longer periods of time (Romanovsky et al., 2009). Lastly, we noticed that DTX-treated CGRPα-DTR+/−

mice gradually lost weight over the course of our experiments (using DTX from two different vendors; Figure 6, Figure S5). This appears to be an on-target effect because weight loss did not occur when wild-type mice Selleckchem SB431542 were treated with DTX (Figure S4). This then raises the question of why DTX-treated CGRPα-DTR+/− mice lost weight.

Given that these mice showed enhanced sensitivity to cold, greater tonic activity over in cold-responsive spinal neurons, and preferred warmer temperatures over cooler temperatures, one possibility is that DTX-treated mice tonically “feel” cold and are in a cold-challenged physiological state at room temperature. In such a state, animals metabolize brown fat and other body tissues to generate energy and heat (Romanovsky et al., 2009). Ultimately, additional studies will be needed to determine whether CGRPα DRG neurons regulate energy and fat metabolism in a manner similar to TRPV1 neurons (Motter and Ahern, 2008; Romanovsky et al., 2009). All procedures involving vertebrate animals were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. Cgrpα-GFP−/− female mice ( McCoy et al., 2012) (available from MMRRC:36773) were crossed with male Advillin-Cre−/− mice ( Hasegawa et al., 2007) to generate double heterozygous CGRPα+/−; Advillin-Cre+/− (CGRPα-DTR+/−) mice. Heterozygous offspring were used for all experiments and have one functional Calca allele. All mice were backcrossed to C57BL/6 mice for at least eight generations. Mice were raised on a 12 hr:12 hr light:dark cycle, were fed DietGel 76A (72-03-502, ClearH2O) and water ad libitum, and were tested during the light phase. Estrous cycle was not monitored in females.

Runners who CRFS or CRFS when both barefoot and shod have been st

Runners who CRFS or CRFS when both barefoot and shod have been studied previously. 5, 9, 14, 16 and 17 However, almost half of the runners in the current study shifted their running style between an RFS when shod and an FFS when barefoot. 12 Within a given group or footwear condition, an increase in speed increases both stride length and stride frequency (Table 1).11, 15 and 20 Barefoot runners generally run with shorter stride lengths and higher stride frequencies and

are more likely to FFS than shod runners (Fig. 3).3, 11, 15, 16 and 20 Shorter stride lengths attenuate the shock wave caused by the heel strike at initial contact2 and may also reduce decelerations that occur within running strides, due to more vertical ground reaction forces.26 Interestingly, CFFS and CRFS runners

used similar stride lengths and frequencies at a given speed and footwear condition (Fig. 3). Shod shifters, however, Protein Tyrosine Kinase inhibitor use longer PI3K inhibitor stride lengths and higher stride frequencies and duty cycles than all other groups (Fig. 3). Runners who change their running style also modulate their stride length and stride frequency. Notably, runners with consistent styles, whether FFS or RFS, have stride lengths, stride frequencies and duty cycles similar to each other across groups. The similar stride lengths, frequencies, and duty cycles between CFFS and CRFS runners may relate more to training level than foot strike pattern. Sometimes, training shortens stride lengths27 and 28 but elite first training lengthens stride lengths.29 In our subjects, even though the level of training and mileage did not differ between the three groups, these effects may mask differences in stride length. When training level is controlled (e.g., in the shifters), FFS barefoot runners shorten their stride lengths compared to the RFS shod runners (Fig. 3).11, 16 and 20 Only the shod shifters ran with longer stride lengths (Fig. 3). These shod runners may be using the cushioning of the shoe to attenuate the

increased shock experienced through the leg and decrease energy absorption when running with longer stride lengths.2 Shorter strides during FFS running also correlate with more vertical landing angles (less overstride; Fig. 4).3 FFS runners land with their shank more vertical (2°) compared to RFS runners (8°). Barefoot RFS runners also land with a more vertical shank compared to shod RFS runners.16 This vertical landing angle in FFS runners likely functions to minimize ankle moments.2 FFS runners slightly plantarflex (−12.5°) their ankle joints at impact, RFS runners slightly dorsiflex their ankle joints (1.2°; Fig. 4 and Fig. 5),3, 11, 12, 13, 14 and 19 while shifters alter their kinematics to correspond to the two styles of running. Typically, running involves a quick plantarflexion at heel contact before dorsiflexion,2, 4, 16, 26, 30, 31 and 32 but only for RFS runners (Fig. 4).